I ain't afraid of no ghosts: people with mind-blindness not so easily spooked
The link between mental imagery and emotions may be closer than we thought.
The link between mental imagery and emotions may be closer than we thought.
Sherry Landow
News & Content Producer
(02) 9065 4039
s.landow@unsw.edu.au
People with 鈥 that is, the inability to visualise mental images 鈥 are harder to spook with scary stories, a new UNSW Sydney study shows.听
The study, published today in , tested how aphantasic people reacted to reading distressing scenarios, like being chased by a shark, falling off a cliff, or being in a plane that鈥檚 about to crash.
The researchers were able to physically measure each participant鈥檚 fear response by monitoring changing skin conductivity levels 鈥 in other words, how much the story made a person sweat. This type of test is commonly used in psychology research to measure the body鈥檚 physical expression of emotion.听
According to the findings, scary stories lost their fear factor when the readers couldn鈥檛 visually imagine the scene 鈥 suggesting imagery may have a closer link to emotions than scientists previously thought.
鈥淲e found the strongest evidence yet that mental imagery plays a key role in linking thoughts and emotions,鈥 says Professor , senior author on the paper and Director of UNSW Science鈥檚 .听
鈥淚n all of our research to date, this is by far the biggest difference we鈥檝e found between people with aphantasia and the general population.鈥
Finding yourself trapped in a room full of spiders 鈥 and feeling them slowly crawl over you 鈥 was one of the scary stories used in the experiment. Photo: Unsplash.
To test the role of visual imagery in fear, the researchers guided 46 study participants (22 with aphantasia, and 24 with imagery) to a blackened room before attaching several electrodes to their skin. Skin is known to become a better conductor of electricity when a person feels strong emotions, like fear.
The scientists then left the room and turned the light off, leaving the participants alone as a story started to appear in the screen in front of them.听聽
At first, the stories started innocuously 鈥 for example, 鈥榊ou are at the beach, in the water鈥 or 鈥榊ou鈥檙e on a plane, by the window鈥. But as the stories continued, the suspense slowly built, whether it was a dark flash in the distant waves and people on the beach pointing, or the cabin lights dimming as the plane starts to shake.听
鈥淪kin conductivity levels quickly started to grow for people who were able to visualise the stories,鈥 says Prof Pearson. 鈥淭he more the stories went on, the more their skin reacted.
鈥淏ut for people with aphantasia, the skin conductivity levels pretty much flatlined.鈥
To check that differences in fear thresholds didn鈥檛 cause the response, the experiment was repeated using a series of scary images instead of text, like a photo of a cadaver or a snake bearing its fangs.
But this time, the pictures made the skin crawl equally in both groups of people.
鈥淭hese two sets of results suggest that aphantasia isn鈥檛 linked to reduced emotion in general, but is specific to participants reading scary stories,鈥 says Prof. Pearson. 鈥淭he emotional fear response was present when participants actually saw the scary material play out in front of them.
鈥淭he findings suggest that imagery is an emotional thought amplifier. We can think all kind of things, but without imagery, the thoughts aren鈥檛 going to have that emotional 鈥榖oom鈥.鈥
Do you want to hear a scary story? Photo: Shutterstock
Aphantasia affects 2-5 per cent of the population, but there is still very little known about the condition.听
A UNSW study published last year found that aphantasia is linked to a , like remembering, dreaming and imagining.
But while most previous aphantasia research focused on behavioural studies, this study used an objective measure of skin conductance.
鈥淭his evidence further supports aphantasia as a unique, verifiable phenomenon,鈥 says study co-author Dr Rebecca Keogh, a postdoctoral fellow formerly of UNSW and now based at Macquarie University.
鈥淭his work may provide a potential new objective tool which could be used to help to confirm and diagnose aphantasia in the future.鈥
The idea for this experiment came after the research team noticed a recurring sentiment on aphantasia discussion boards that many people with the condition didn鈥檛 enjoy reading fiction.
While the findings suggest that reading may not be as emotionally impactful for people with aphantasia, Prof. Pearson says it鈥檚 important to note that the findings are based on averages, and not everyone with aphantasia will have the same reading experience.听
The study was also focused on fear, and other emotional responses to fiction could be different.
While some people with aphantasia only experience a lack of visual imagery, others also have trouble imagining other senses, like sounds, tastes, and touch. Photo: Unsplash.
鈥淎phantasia comes in different shapes and sizes,鈥 he says. 鈥淪ome people have no visual imagery, while other people have no imagery in one or all of their other senses. Some people dream while others don鈥檛.
鈥淪o don鈥檛 be concerned if you have aphantasia and don't fit this mould. There are all kinds of variations to aphantasia that we鈥檙e only just discovering.鈥
Next, Prof. Pearson and his team at the Future Minds Lab plan to investigate how disorders like anxiety and Post Traumatic Stress Disorder might be experienced differently by people with aphantasia.
鈥淎phantasia is neural diversity,鈥 says Prof. Pearson. 鈥淚t鈥檚 an amazing example of how different our brain and minds can be.鈥
To be involved in this type of research and learn more about aphantasia and the Future Minds Lab, visit .